Show simple item record

[journal article]

dc.contributor.authorPapies, Esther K.de
dc.contributor.authorAarts, Henkde
dc.contributor.authorVries, Nanne K. dede
dc.date.accessioned2012-06-21T02:53:00Zde
dc.date.accessioned2012-08-29T23:02:31Z
dc.date.available2012-08-29T23:02:31Z
dc.date.issued2009de
dc.identifier.urihttp://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/31048
dc.description.abstractStudies on implementation intentions so far have mainly pointed towards strengthened cue-behavior associations as the mechanism underlying the effectiveness of this self-regulatory tool. However, we propose that because it triggers people to look into the future and to mentally simulate their future behavior, planning by means of implementation intentions might go beyond the creation of goal-directed associations and thus lead to more enduring effects on behavior. We tested this hypothesis in an experiment using a longitudinal design, where participants formed an intention for a behavior that deviates from their routine, and furnished it either with associative learning of cue and behavior, forming implementation intentions, or nothing at all. Results showed that initially, learning cue-behavior associations led to the same rate of goal completion as forming implementation intentions. However, only the effect of implementation intentions was maintained at the second measurement one week later. These findings suggest that planning does more than merely create goal-directed associations, which might offer a new perspective on the workings and use of this important tool for behavior change.en
dc.languageende
dc.subject.ddcPsychologyen
dc.subject.ddcPsychologiede
dc.subject.otherbehavior change; implementation intentions; self-regulation; goal-directed behavior; action planning; social cognition
dc.titlePlanning is for doing: implementation intentions go beyond the mere creation of goal-directed associationsen
dc.description.reviewbegutachtet (peer reviewed)de
dc.description.reviewpeer revieweden
dc.source.journalJournal of Experimental Social Psychologyde
dc.source.volume45de
dc.source.issue5de
dc.subject.classozSocial Psychologyen
dc.subject.classozSozialpsychologiede
dc.identifier.urnurn:nbn:de:0168-ssoar-310483de
dc.date.modified2012-06-21T09:14:00Zde
dc.rights.licencePEER Licence Agreement (applicable only to documents from PEER project)de
dc.rights.licencePEER Licence Agreement (applicable only to documents from PEER project)en
ssoar.gesis.collectionSOLIS;ADISde
ssoar.contributor.institutionhttp://www.peerproject.eu/de
internal.status3de
dc.type.stockarticlede
dc.type.documentjournal articleen
dc.type.documentZeitschriftenartikelde
dc.rights.copyrightfde
dc.source.pageinfo1148-1151
internal.identifier.classoz10706
internal.identifier.journal199de
internal.identifier.document32
internal.identifier.ddc150
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.011de
dc.description.pubstatusPostprinten
dc.description.pubstatusPostprintde
internal.identifier.licence7
internal.identifier.pubstatus2
internal.identifier.review1
internal.check.abstractlanguageharmonizerCERTAIN
internal.check.languageharmonizerCERTAIN_RETAINED


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record